Dogs

Dog Calming Music

Music Speakers for Dogs

All Dog Products

Cats

Cat Calming Music

Music Speakers for Cats

All Cat Products

People

People Calming Music

Music Speakers for People

All People Products

M

Dogs

Dog Calming Music

Music Speakers for Dogs

All Dog Products

Cats

Cat Calming Music

Music Speakers for Cats

All Cat Products

People

People Calming Music

Music Speakers for People

All People Products

The Music

Why Music for Pets?

Why Music for People?

How Our Music Works

Psychoacoustics

Research

Take a Sonic Inventory of Your Sound Environment

Company

Joshua Leeds, Sound Researcher

Bioacoustic Research

The Musicians

Shelter Program

Wholesale

iCalmPet Blog

6 Ways to Prepare Your Pets for the Holidays

5 Tips to Keep Your Pets Comfortable and Safe During Winter

Separation Anxiety And Your Dog: The Complete Guide

5 Recipes to Help You Create Your Own Dog Treats at Home

3 Ways to Make Exercising Fun with Your Pet

View iCalmPet Blog
M

The Music

Why Music for Pets?

Why Music for People?

How Our Music Works

Psychoacoustics

Research

Take a Sonic Inventory of Your Sound Environment

Company

Joshua Leeds, Sound Researcher

Bioacoustic Research

The Musicians

Shelter Program

Wholesale

iCalmPet Blog

6 Ways to Prepare Your Pets for the Holidays

Together All the Time: At Home with Pets During Covid-19

Keeping Your Dog Occupied at Home During Quarantine

Hearing Loss in Dogs 2020 [Causes + Solutions]

Separation Anxiety And Your Dog: The Complete Guide

View iCalmPet Blog

Technical Support

Product Instructions

Download Assistance

Lithium-Ion Battery Tips

Customer Service

Payment & Shipping

Warranty

Returns

Your Privacy

Common FAQs

Why music for noise phobias?

What if I have a dog AND a cat?

Does the iCalmPet speaker come with a guarantee?

Can I use iCalm for myself, or is it only for pets?

View all FAQs

M

Technical Support

Product Instructions

Download Assistance

Lithium-Ion Battery Tips

Customer Service

Payment & Shipping

Warranty

Returns

Your Privacy

Common FAQs

Why music for noise phobias?

What if I have a dog AND a cat?

Does the iCalmPet speaker come with a guarantee?

Can I use iCalm for myself, or is it only for pets?

View all FAQs

Contact Us

This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.

Contact Info

Mail:
iCalmPet
1467 Siskiyou Blvd, #30
Ashland, OR 97520 USA

Phone:
9:00am – 12:00pm PST, M-F
(800) 788-0949 (USA only)
(541) 482-2134
Fax: (541) 488-7796

Connect with Us

M

Contact Info

Mail:
iCalmPet
1467 Siskiyou Blvd, #30
Ashland, OR 97520 USA

Phone:
9:00am – 12:00pm PST, M-F
(800) 788-0949 (USA only)
(541) 482-2134
Fax: (541) 488-7796

Connect with Us

Recent research into the effect of music on dogs (cats, monkeys) includes the following studies:

  1. 2002 Wells, D. L., et al. “The Influence of Auditory Stimulation on the Behaviour of Dogs Housed in a Rescue Shelter.” Animal Welfare 11 (2002): 385-393
  2. 2004 Wagner, S., et al. “Canine Research Summary.” Bioacoustic Research & Development Executive Summary (2004)
  3. 2009 Snowden, C., Teie, D., et al.”Affective Responses in Tamarins Elicited by Species-Specific Music.” Animal Behaviour, Royal Society Publishing (2009)
  4. 2012  Kogan, L., et al. “Behavioral Effects of Auditory Stimulation on Kennelled Dogs.” Journal of Veterinary Behavior 5 (2012) 268-275
  5. 2015 Bowman, A., et al. “‘Four Searsons’ in an Animal Rescue Centre; Classical Music Reduces Environmental Stress in Kennelled Dogs.” Physiology & Behavior 143 (2015) 70-82
  6. 2015 Snowden, C., Teie, D., et al. “Cats Prefer Species-Appropriate Music.” Applied Animal Behavior Science 166 (2015) 106-111
  7. 2016 Brayley, C., et al. The Effects of Audiobooks on the Behavior of Dogs at a Rehoming Kennel.” Applied Animal Behavior Science 174 (2016) 111-115
  8. 2017  Bains, M., et al. “Effect of Different Types of Classical Music Played at a Veterinary Hospital on Dog Behavior and Owner Satisfaction.” JAVMA 2 (2017) 195-200
  9. 2017 Bowman, A., et al. “The Effect of Different Genres of Music on the Stress Levels of Kennelled Dogs.” Physiology & Behavior 171 (2017) 207-215

Notes on the inter-relationship of  research studies #1 (Wells) and #2 (Wagner):

Dr. Deborah Wells, Belfast, Ireland In 2002, Belfast-based psychologist and animal behaviorist Deborah Wells undertook a research program to determine the influence of five types of auditory stimulation: human conversation, classical music, heavy metal music, pop music, and a silent control (no music at all).

From Dr. Wells’s study, we came to understand that classical music had a marked soothing effect on dogs in animal shelters when compared to the other types of auditory stimulation. In the discussion section of her published research, Dr. Wells stated, “Classical music resulted in dogs spending more of their time resting than any of the other experimental conditions of auditory stimulation. This type of music also resulted in a significantly lower level of barking.

Research suggests that calming music may have a beneficial effect on humans, resulting in diminished agitation, improved mood and lower levels of stress. Although the specific effect of classical music on dogs remains unknown, the findings from this study suggest that it may, as in humans, have a calming influence.” She concluded that heavy metal agitated the dogs, indicated by increased frequencies of standing and barking, and that neither human conversation nor pop music had any apparent effect on the dog’s behaviors, perhaps due to habituation to radio exposure.

Dr. Wells stated, “Further work is still required to unravel the specific acoustic elements that dogs respond to.” That challenge inspired us to take our bioacoustic research where no one had gone before.

The Bioacoustic Research & Development Project, USA

Understanding that some things about classical music were having an optimum effect on dogs, we endeavored to take Deborah Wells’s work one step further. The purpose of our BioAcoustic Research & Development project was to investigate the effects of multiple types of classical music on the behavior of dogs in kennel and home environments. The music was chosen and arranged according to the principles of entrainment (beats per minute) and harmonic complexity (active listening versus passive hearing). We know that dogs have the same brain-wave patterns as humans. However, dogs’ heart rates vary according to size; the larger the dog, the slower the heart rate. The tempos used in this project were based on an average-size dog.

We also recognized that domesticated animals possess more highly tuned hearing than people, yet we did not know if animal cerebral function would allow them to recognize sonic relationships (i.e., intervals, harmonies, and fast or slow external rhythms). In short, effectuating change in the human nervous system with sound and music is known, but what would be the result using the same principles on dogs?

After highly specific recordings were made, we created two pilot studies, involving more than 150 dogs. Between June 2004 and September 2005, these dogs were observed while listening to different combinations of the recordings Lisa Spector made for the project, as well as the control CDs, which consisted of non-psychoacoustically- arranged classical music.

Two Pilot Studies

Pilot I. The purpose of Pilot I was to determine the efficacy of external rhythm and pattern identification on canines in the kennel and home environment. Four albums of psychoacoustically designed classical music were tested — two albums of solo piano at varying tempos and two albums of piano trios, also at varying tempos. The results suggest that all classical music doesn’t have the same effect on behavior in dogs:

  • In the kennel environment, over 70 percent of the dogs became calmer with the simplified, 50-60 beats per minute (bpm) — both solo piano and trio music.
  • In the home environment, the solo piano at 50-60 bpm showed an average of 85 percent becoming calm, and over half the dogs went to sleep.
Pilot I Summary Results

Instrumentation and tempo of the classical music can produce marked differences in results. Solo instruments, slower tempos, and less complex arrangements had a greater calming effect than faster selections with more complex harmonic and orchestral content.

Pilot II. The purpose of Pilot II was to determine if music arranged according to psychoacoustic principles would have an effect on specific anxiety issues in dogs, such as fear of separation, thunderstorms, and fireworks. Upon review of the data from Pilot I, it was determined that the simple and slow piano CD showed the most consistent results for calming dogs in both kennel and home environments. Because many guardians turn the radio on for their pets when they leave home or when a thunderstorm is approaching, another CD of standard classical music was chosen for comparison. This music was taken from the play list of a San Francisco classical radio station and was not psychoacoustically rearranged.

Ten dogs with anxiety were used in Pilot II.
Their specific anxieties stemmed from the following:

  • Other dogs or children
  • Visitors in the home environment
  • Thunderstorms
  • Riding in the car
  • Excessive need for attention — pawing at guardian
  • Separation anxiety
  • Fireworks
Pilot II Summary Results

Results showed 70 percent of anxiety behaviors were reduced with psychoacoustically designed music, while 36 percent of anxiety behaviors were reduced with the non-psychoacoustic control CD. Both CDs calmed the dogs enough to make them lie down. However, it appears that the psychoacoustically designed music, with slower tempo and simpler arrangements and sounds, is more effective in reducing anxiety.

Click here to view the 2004 analysis of the BARD dog studies (PDF).

Comments on the BARD study from Joshua Leeds, co-founder and producer of iCalmPet:

What is fascinating, from a psychoacoustician’s viewpoint, is the fact that dogs were reacting in a similar manner to music that had been created for specific effects on people. In other words, dogs could be entrained, i.e., heart rate, brain waves, and breath slowed or speeded up when influenced by external rhythms, just as in people. It is also intriguing to note that the complexity of sound affected dogs as it does people: the more complexity in the music, the more energy required to decipher it. Likewise, the simpler the sound, the greater the relaxation response.

The fact that this simple pattern identification process had an effect on dogs with anxiety issues is even more startling for it suggests that dogs suffer from over-stimulation in the same way that people do. Stimulus is defined as “something that produces a temporary increase of physiological activity.”

We know from the extraordinary work of Dr. Alfred Tomatis that high-pitched (treble) sounds will stimulate and lower sounds (bass) de-stimulate the nervous system. (The first 80 percent of hearing cells in the inner ear process mid-high frequency tones. The remaining 20 percent of the cells process mid-low sounds. Dr. Tomatis discovered that if the ear is overwhelmed with too many low sounds, fatigue occurs and the brain does not receive the full benefit of the sonic energy. High sounds, on the other hand, create optimal stimulation.

Of the five CDs used in the combined studies, the single CD with the most startling results was one designed specifically for human calmness. This CD is the first in the Through a Dog’s Ear series. Entrainment — the process whereby our internal pulses will match a periodic rhythm — is easily observed in people who are tapping their toes and moving their heads to a rhythm. As we noted earlier, we have never witnessed a dog that pants or wags its tail to a beat. However, the results of these studies show that even without physical demonstration of rhythmic entrainment, the internal organs are still speeding up or slowing down to match external rhythmic stimuli. The ramifications of this are great. Our canine companions are totally impacted by the sonic landscapes in which we surround them — and ourselves.